Thomas Perera Ph.D. - Professor of Psychology

Author of COMPUTER PROGRAMS FOR EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY.

Author of MODULAR LABORATORY SESSIONS FOR PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY AND GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY

     The COMPUTER PROGRAMS FOR EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY allow desktop 
computers to perform most of the classical experiments in the field 
of psychology without the need for specialized apparatus. 

     The MODULAR LABORATORY SESSIONS FOR PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY may 
be used in several ways. They may be used to show students how to 
perform complex surgical procedures before they actually attempt them.  
They may also be used as an alternative to having students perform the 
procedures to avoid unnecessary use of animals or in cases where the 
educational institution lacks the equipment or experience to permit 
their completion.  The MODULAR LABORATORY SESSIONS FOR GENERAL 
PSYCHOLOGY may be used as instructional adjuncts for courses in
general psychology, experimental psychology, and perception.

Descriptions of each of the 27 computer programs are presented below.
They are followed by descriptions of the 20 modular laboratory sessions.

X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 
For further information, and demonstration disks, 
please contact the publisher:

LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES
1 Fenimore Road
Bayport, NY 11705
(516) 472-2111
e-mail: lifesciassoc@pipeline.com
web pages: lifesciassoc.home.pipeline.com
X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X COMPUTER PROGRAMS FOR EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY These are a set of 27 computer programs written in the BASIC computer language and designed to allow students and researchers to perform most of the classical experiments in the field of psychology on a desktop computer. Since they are written in BASIC, they can easily be modified and customized to perform experiments for undergraduate, graduate, and faculty research. The programs eliminate the need for the specialized dedicated apparatus usually required in a psychological laboratory X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Brief descriptions of each of the programs are given below: INDEX OF PROGRAMS: 1. Reaction Time 14. Latency Analyzer 2. Muller Lyer 15. Interresponse Time Analyzer 3. Horizontal/Vertical 16. Event Recorder 4. Poggendorf 17. Cumulative Recorder 5. Line Length 18. Human Maze Learning 6. Rectangle Size 19. Signal Detection 7. Concept Formation 20. Pursuit Rotor 8. Verbal Learning 21. Mirror Tracing 9. Tachistoscope 22. Auditory Freq Difference Thresholds 10. Visual Illusions 23. Auditory Frequency Scaling 11. Psychophysical Scaling 24. Laboratory Control System 12. Visual Acuity 25. Operant Conditioning Control 13. Operant conditioning *26. Delayed Matching to Sample * *27. Levels of Processing * * Written by Dr. John A. Brendel X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 1. REACTION TIME (602) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Reaction time, the time elapsing between a stimulus and a response, was one of the first dependent variables to be used in psychological experimentation. This is an exceptionally versatile and complete reaction time program that allows measurement of the speed of a human subject's responses to stimuli flashed on the computer screen. From one to ten stimuli can be presented and from one to ten responses recorded. Number of trials, intetertrial intervals, ready signal, and randomized foreperiods can be selected from an interactive display prior to beginning the experiment. The program controls for premature responses and excessively long latencies. At the end of the selected number of trials, the available data analysis options include mean, SD, variance, standard error and coefficient of variation. The reaction times for each foreperiod and stimulus are individually statistically analyzed and T- tests may be displayed to compare foreperiods or stimuli. Raw data can be displayed, a data file can be made, more trials may be added to existing data, or the experiment may be rerun. Many of the Classical reaction-time experiments can be replicated, and a voice key or other external manipulandum may be used by attaching it to the computer keyboard key contacts. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the system and describes how to make modifications in the system for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 2. THE MULLER-LYER ILLUSION (604) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The Muller-Lyer Illusion has been studied for well over 100 years. In this illusion, two equal length lines appear of unequal length when arrowheads are placed at their ends. To date, no fully satisfactory explanation of the phenomenon has been proposed. To study the effect, it must be quantified along some numerical continuum so that changes produced by varying the stimulus or organismic variables can be detected. Any of the classical psychophysical methods may be used for quantifying the Muller-Lyer Illusion. The Muller-Lyer program allows the computer system to present the Muller-Lyer Illusion using one of the three classical psychophysical methods. The experimenter may select the method of adjustment, method of limits, or method of constant stimuli. The experimenter may also select the number of trials to be run and the maximum duration of each trial. The subject is then presented with a set of instructions followed by presentations of the Muller-Lyer Illusion. The subject's responses, depressions of keyboard keys indicating judgments of shorter, equal, or longer, are categorized in the memory of the computer during the running of the experiment. At the end of the experiment, data analysis options include viewing a statistical analysis of the data which includes values for the point of subjective equality, constant error, difference limen, and interval' of uncertainty. Probabilities and Z-scores for each stimulus are displayed with the method of constant stimuli. Other options include the display of the raw points of subjective equality for each trial, making a recording of the data for future analysis or long term storage, adding more trials to the present data, or running another experiment. Instructions describe how the experiment works and how to make changes in various aspects of the experiment. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 3. QUANTIFICATION OF THE HORIZONTAL-VERTICAL ILLUSION (606) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The Horizontal-Vertical Illusion has been studied for well over 100 years. In this illusion, a vertical line appears longer than a horizontal line even though the two lines are actually the same length. To date, no fully satisfactory explanation of the phenomenon has been proposed. To study the effect, it must be quantified along some numerical continuum so that changes produced by varying the stimulus or organismic variables can be detected. Any of the classical psychophysical methods may be used for quantifying the Horizontal-Vertical Illusion. The Horizontal-Vertical program allows the researcher to study the Horizontal-Vertical Illusion using one of the three classical psychophysical methods. The experimenter may select the method of adjustment, method of limits, or method of constant stimuli. The experimenter may also select the number of trials to be run and the maximum duration of each trial. The subject is then presented with a set of instructions followed by presentations of the Horizontal-Vertical Illusion. The subject's responses, depressions of keyboard keys indi- cating judgments of shorter, equal, or longer, are categorized in the memory of the computer during the running of the experiment. At the end of the experiment, data analysis options include viewing a statistical analysis of the data which includes values for the point of sub-jective equality, constant error, difference limen, and interval of uncertainty. Probabilities and Z-scores for each stimulus are displayed with the method of constant stimuli. Other options include the display of the raw points of subjective equality for each trial, making a disk recording of the data for future analysis or long term storage, adding more trials to the present data, or running another experiment. Instructions describe how the experiment works and how to make changes in various aspects of the experiment. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 4. QUANTIFICATION OF THE POGGENDORF ILLUSION (608) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The Poggendorf Illusion has been studied for well over 100 years. It consists of two vertical lines with a diagonal line running through them. The diagonal line which is actually straight appears to have a definite offset. To date, no fully satisfactory explanation of the phenomenon has been proposed. To study the effect, it must be quantified along some numerical continuum so that changes produced by varying the stimulus or organismic variables can be detected. Any of the classical psychophysical methods may be used for quantifying the Poggendorf Illusion. The Poggendorf program allows the researcher to present the Poggendorf Illusion using one of the three classical psychophysical methods. The experimenter may select the method of adjustment, method of limits, or method of constant stimuli. The experimenter may also select the number of trials to be run and the maximum duration of each trial. The subject is then presented with a set of instructions followed by presentations of the illusion. The subject's responses, depressions of keyboard keys indicating judgments of shorter, equal, or longer, are categorized in the memory of the computer during the running of the experiment. At the end of the experiment, data analysis options include viewing a statistical analysis of the data which includes values for the point of subjective equality, constant error, difference limen, and interval of uncertainty. Probabilities and Z-scores for each stimulus are displayed with the method of constant stimuli. Other options include the display of the raw of subjective equality for each trial, making a disk recording of the data for future analysis or long term storage, adding more trials to the present data, or running another experiment. The instructions describe how the experiment works and how to make changes in various aspects of the experiment. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 5. QUANTIFICATION OF LINE LENGTH JUDGMENTS (610) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Quantification of judgments of the apparent length of lines can easily be performed by using one of the classical psychophysical methods. Articles describing the classical psychophysical methods and past research on the line length judgments may be found in virtually any textbook on experimental psychology or perception. A representative list of references is included at the end of the instructions. The line length judgment program allows the researcher to study line length judgments using one of the three classical psychophysical methods. The experimenter may select the method of adjustment, method of limits, or method of constant stimuli. The experimenter may also select the number of trials to be run and the maximum duration of each trial. The subject is then presented with a set of instructions followed by presentations of the line length stimuli. The subject's responses, depressions of keyboard keys indicating judgments of shorter, equal, or longer, are categorized in the memory of the computer during the running of the experiment. At the end of the experiment, data analysis options include viewing a sta-tistical analysis of the data which includes values for the point of subjective equality, constant error, difference limen, and interval of uncertainty. Probabilities and Z-scores for each stimulus are displayed with the method of constant stimuli. Other options include the display of the raw points of subjective equality for each trial, making a disk recording of the data for future analysis or long term storage, adding more trials to the present data, or running another experiment. The instructions included with the program describe how the experiment works and how to make changes in various aspects of the experiment. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 6. QUANTIFICATION OF SIZE OF RECTANGLE JUDGMENTS (611) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Quantification of size judgments can easily be performed using one of the classical psychophysical methods. Articles describing the classical psychophysical methods and past research on size judgments may be found in virtually any textbook on experimental psychology or perception. A representative list of references is included in the instructions. The size of rectangle program allows the researcher to study size judgments using one of the three classical psychophysical methods. The experimenter may select the method of adjustment, method of limits, or method of constant stimuli. The experimenter may also select the number of trials to be run and the maximum duration of each trial. The subject is then presented with a set of instructions followed by presentations of the stimulus figure. The subject's responses, depressions of keyboard keys indicating judgments of shorter, equal, or longer, are categorized in the memory of the computer during the running of the experiment. At the end of the experiment, data analysis options include viewing a statistical analysis of the data which includes values for the point of subjective equality, constant error, difference limen, and interval of uncertainty. Probabilities and Z-scores for each stimulus are displayed with the method of constant stimuli. Other options include the display of the raw points of subjective equality for each trial, making a recording of the data for future analysis or long term storage, adding more trials to the present data, or running another experiment. The instructions included with the program describe how the experiment works and how to make changes in various aspects of the experiment. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 7. CONCEPT FORMATION (612) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Concept formation or, as it is often called, concept attainment, concept identification, or concept learning has been studied by experimental psychologists for many years. In most cases, the experimenter sequentially presents numerous examples of several concepts until the subject has learned which label applies to which concept. Knowledge of results is given as feedback to assist the learning. Most experimental psychology textbooks describe the classical experiments in this area and several typical references are included with the instructions that accompany the program. The concept formation program first presents a set of general instructions to the experimenter. It then allows the experimenter to select preset or externally programmed experimental parameters> It then presents instructions for the subject. The experiment then presents sets consisting of 9 concepts. The subject must complete each set perfectly, a selectable number of times before being permitted to continue on to the next set. The experiment is complete when the subject has completed all of the 6 sets. There are 54 different data analysis options which are available to the experimenter after the completion of the experiment. They include statistical analysis of the data, graphing of the data, and recording the data for future analysis. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to make modifications for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 8. VERBAL LEARNING (614) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Two of the most common types of verbal learning experiments involve serial and paired associates procedures. These procedures, as well as detailed histories, of the field of verbal learning can be found in virtually any textbook on experimental psychology. Several representative texts are listed in the instructions that are supplied with the program. The Verbal Learning program allows both serial and paired associates experiments to be run. The experimenter can select not only the type of experiment to be run, but also whether to use stimulus words that are stored in the program, or to enter stimulus words directly from the keyboard. The experimenter may also select a number of parameters for the experiment before beginning to collect data. Appropriate instructions are then displayed, and the subject is presented with the list or lists of words to be learned. After one pass through the list, the subject is given a cue to type the word, and the subject's response is timed. After the subject has correctly reproduced the learned verbal material a selectable criterion number of times, the program enters the data analysis phase. There are 36 different types of data analysis which can be selected. They include statistical analysis of the data, graphing the data, and recording the data in a file for future analysis. The instructions explain the internal workings of the program and suggest ways to modify it for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 9. MULTIPLE FIELD TACHISTOSCOPE (616) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. This program allows the computer system to function as a multiple field tachistoscope. The computer is capable of presenting a series of stimuli to the subject and measuring the latencies of response to one or more of these stimuli. Tachistoscopes that present multiple visual fields to a subject and measure the response latencies are used in a wide variety of laboratory experiments in psychology. Virtually any textbook in experimental psychology or perception describes the uses, applications, and classical experiments performed with tachistoscopes. A number of representative references are included in the instructions. The tachistoscope program can select the duration of each stimulus, and time the latencies of responses to each stimulus. Each trial begins with the display of a fixation point for a selected duration. The stimuli are then presented in sequence and latencies are measured. At the end of the trial, an inter-trial interval is timed and then the next trial begins. After a preselected number of trials has been completed, the program enters the data analysis phase. During this phase, the experimenter may select from 7 data analysis options. The statistical analysis of the latency data may be displayed, the raw data may be displayed, a data recording may be made, the latency histogram may be displayed, another series of trials may be added to the existing data, the experiment may be rerun with the current parameters, or the experiment may be rerun with the preset parameters. A special calibration routine at the beginning of the program allows the user to set the speed of the program to the speed of any computer. A set of instructions describes ways of modifying the program for special requirements and explains the inner workings of the program. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 10. VISUAL ILLUSION DEMONSTRATIONS (618) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The visual illusion demonstration program allows the computer system to display ten of the classical visual illusions. Many of these illusions have been known for well over 100 years, and yet are still difficult or impossible to explain. Virtually any textbook on experimental psychology or perception deals with the history of, and attempts to explain, visual illusions. A list of references is included at the end of the instructions. The visual illusion program allows the user to select from among 10 visual illusions. The illusions are displayed on the video monitor screen for as long as desired. New illusions may be selected at any time. These instructions include a description of the program's operation and a detailed description of the BASIC language program so that modifications of the illusions may be made. Illusion First described by: Date 1. Muller-Lyer F. C. Muller-Lyer 1889 2. Horizontal-Vertical A. Fick 1851 3. Poggendorf J. C. Poggendorf 1896 4. Ponzo M. Ponzo 1912 5. Necker Cube L. A. Necker 1832 6. Distorted Square W. D. Orbison 1939 7. Distorted Square (2) A. Gatti 1926 8. Sander Parallelogram F. Sander 1926 9. Phi-Phenomenon M. Wertheimer 1912 K. Duncker 1938 10. Motion/Causality A. Michotte 1946 X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 11. PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF LINE LENGTH (620) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Psychophysical scaling is a technique which allows a subject to assign a numerical value to the subjective magnitude of a stimulus. When these subjective judgments are plotted against the physical magnitude of the stimulus, a psychophysical function is produced. The shape of the psychophysical function has been a source of argument for many years. Fechner's law predicts that the psychological judgments are linearly related the logarithm of the stimulus. Fechner thus predicts that plotting the psychological judgments on the vertical axis against the logarithm of the stimulus values on the horizontal axis will yield a straight line. Stevens' law predicts a straight line when the logarithm of the psychological judgments is plotted on the vertical axis and the logarithm of the physical stimulus is plotted on the horizontal axis. Detailed discussions of psychophysical scaling procedures and techniques can be found in virtually all textbooks on experimental psychology and psychophysics. A representative list of such textbooks is attached to the end of the instructions. The psychophysical scaling of line length program allows the computer system to accept and analyze numerous psychophysical judgments of the lengths of lines that are presented on the video screen. After a preselected number of trials has elapsed, the data analysis options include drawing a graph of the individual data points or the averaged data points, drawing a graph of the best fit least squares line through these points or drawing a graph of both the points and the best fit line. Each of these three graphs may be drawn on linear, common log, natural log, or exponential axes. If the graphs are drawn with the Y axis plotted in common logarithms, and the X axis plotted linearly, this graph corresponds to Fechner's law ( Y = log X + K ). If the Y axis and the X axis are both plotted as common logarithms, this corresponds to Steven's law ( log Y = log X + K ). The program allows the data to be plotted in both ways and compared for linearity. Other data analysis options include displaying or printing the raw data sequentially on a judgment-by-judgment basis, making a sequential judgment-by-judgment data recording, adding more trials up to a maximum total of 40 (10 judgment) trials, restarting the program at the beginning, or stopping the program. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to make modifications in the program for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 12. VISUAL ACUITY (622) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Visual acuity is a measure of the ability of the eye to correctly identify fine detail. The higher the visual acuity, the smaller the stimulus that can be correctly identified. Visual acuity is defined as the reciprocal of the threshold visual angle. The threshold visual angle is the minimum angle subtended by the stimulus at the subject's eye at which the stimulus can be correctly identified. Visual acuity is greatest in the central portion of the eye or fovea, and decreases gradually with distance from the fovea. Virtually any textbook on experimental psychology covers the topic of visual acuity and describes its measurement. Some appropriate references are included in the porgram instructions. The visual acuity program allows the computer system to measure and plot a subject's visual acuity at various retinal locations. Visual acuity is classically measured with a device called a perimeter. The subject fixates a small point directly in form of the eye while a small visual stimulus, typically a gap in a circle called a Landolt "C", is moved in toward the fixation point. The location in the subject's visual field, at which the subject can just determine that the gap exists, is recorded and finally a plot of these points is made. This plot represents an iso-acuity plot and shows the locations in the subject's visual field where the sub-ject's acuity is the same. This program allows determination of the threshold visual angle at various points in the subject's visual field by measuring where the subject can just see a 3 mm gap in the circle. The visual angle of the 3 mm gap is calculated by the formula: Visual angle (degrees) = 57.3 X size of gap (mm) / distance of gap from eye (mm) Since the size of the gap is fixed at 3 mm, the visual angle may be varied by varying the distance from the subject's eye to the video screen. For example: Distance from eye to screen (mm) Visual angle (degrees) Acuity 150 1.146 .873 200 .860 .1163 250 .688 1.454 300 .573 1.745 350 .491 2.036 The experimenter must select the desired acuity value from this table and position the subject's head accurately at the prescribed distance from the video screen. The subject's head should be stabilized in such a way that little or no movement toward or away from the screen will take place during the experiment. The subject's eye for which the acuity is to be measured should be located directly in front of the center of the video screen at the correct distance from the screen. The other eye should be covered with an eye patch. NOTE: since different video monitors produce different size characters, you should measure the size of the gap in the capital letter "C" on your monitor and use this measurement to calculate acuity if it is different from the usual 3mm size. Further instructions for refining and modifying the program are included. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 13. OPERANT CONDITIONING SIMULATION (624) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The operant conditioning program allows the computer system to simulate an entire operant conditioning experiment and to observe and record response rates generated by different mathematical models of response probability. The user may select from among 10 conditioning schedules. Each schedule is associated with a different mathematical model of the probability of a response. A small picture of a rat is then displayed on the video monitor along with the axes of a cumulative response record. The rat presses the bar at a rate predicted by the response probability model and the cumulative record displays each response. The mathematical models of response probability for each schedule of reinforcement are described in detail in the instructions, and may be easily modified to observe the effect of other response probability models. Operant conditioning is described in virtually every textbook dealing with learning. A representative set of references is included in the instructions. This program does not always generate the classical textbook operant conditioning cumulative records. Instead, it generates cumulative response curves that result from a specific mathematical probability-of-response model. If the user wishes to demonstrate simply the textbook cumulative response curves, an 11th schedule option allows the operant conditioning proGram to record depressing of the (1) key on the keyboard as responses on the cumulative re-sponse curve. The user may easily generate any type of cumulative response curve manually by pressing the (1) key or by activating an external input line connected directly to the keyboard contacts of the (1) key. In addition, pressing the (2) or (3) keys or activating other external inputs will mark the occurrence of reinforcements or stimulus presentations respectively. Several data analysis options become available after the cumulative response curve has been completed. These options include reconstructing the cumulative response curve within a few seconds, rescaling its vertical axis, displaying the raw data, making a recording of the data for possible future analysis or long term storage, running the experiment again on the same schedule or on a new schedule, starting the program again, or stopping the program. The instructions explain the use, operation, and possible modifications of the computer program. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 14. LATENCY ANALYZER (626) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The latency analyzer program converts the computer into a timer which is capable of measuring the elapsed time between the onset of a stimulus and the onset of a response. Keyboard keys or external input lines are used to signal the onset of the stimulus and the response. The stimulus number and the latency in sec. are displayed on the video screen after each trial. The user may specify the number of latencies to be measured and the resolution, and may terminate the recording at any time. Data analysis options include displaying a time interval histogram of the data, displaying or printing the raw data, recording the data, or displaying a statistical analysis of the data. Other selectable options include adding another set of data to the existing data, or restarting the experiment with new or old parameters. A complete set of instructions explains the use, internal operation, and modification of the program. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 15. INTERRESPONSE TIME ANALYZER (628) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The interresponse time analyzer program converts the computer into an accurate timing device. The program is designed to measure and record the time intervals elapsing between successive responses. The responses may be either depressions of the (1) key, or activation of an external input wire connected to the electrical keyboard contacts for the (1) key. The user may select the maximum number of responses to be measured and the size of each time bin in the interresponse time histogram. The program will display each latency as it is recorded. Data analysis options include displaying the time interval histogram, displaying, printing or recording the raw data, or a general statistical analysis of the data. The statistical analysis is based on the midpoints of the histogram bins. The program starts out by presenting a brief set of instructions which explain the operation of the program. Each time the (1) key on the keyboard is depressed or an external input line is activated, the program begins timing the elapsed time until the next similar event. These elapsed times are divided into time categories or bins. The experimenter may set the size or resolution of these time bins at the beginning of the experiment. Permissible bin sizes range from 0.1 sec. to 10000 sec. per bin. Since there are always 100 bins in the interresponse time distribution, the maximum measurable interval between responses is 100 times the selected bin size in seconds. It no changes are made, the number of responses is automatically set to 100. The bin width is set to 0.1 sec. This means that the maximum time interval that can be measured is 100 times 0.1, or 10 seconds. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to make modifications in the program for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 16. EVENT RECORDER PROGRAM (630) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. This data logging program allows up to eight independent events to be recorded in the memory of the computer along with the time since the start of the session at which the events occurred. Because it may be desirable to record a large number of events, memory space in the computer is at a premium. The program itself has been kept to a minimum size in order to provide maximum memory space for storage of recorded events. The data analysis options of the program have been restricted to just two. The data may be displayed or printed in raw form, or it may be recorded on a disk for later, more detailed, analysis. Events which are recorded in this program consist of depressions of the keyboard keys 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, or 8. If external events are to be measured, you may connect wires to the contacts on the keyboard so that connecting the wires will electrically mimic depressing the keys. Pressing keyboard key (9) starts and stops data acquisition. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to make modifications in it. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 17. CUMULATIVE RECORDER (632) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. This program allows the computer system to record and display responses in the format of a cumulative recording. The horizontal axis of the recording consists of 600 time units. The vertical axis of the cumulative recorder consists of 160 response units. The amount of time and number of responses per vertical axis unit may be specified during the initial display portions of the program. Responses are entered by either depressing the (1) key or connecting a wire to the contacts of this keyboard key. External contact closures of this wire may come from the normally open contacts of any switch such as a rat's bar. Depression of the (2) key or connecting a wire to the (2) key contacts will record reinforcements on the graph. Depressing the (3) key or connecting a wire to the (3) key contacts will display stimulus presentations on the cumulative record. The data analysis options allow the experimenter to reconstruct and rescale the cumulative record, display or print the raw data, or make a recording of the data in a disk file for long term storage or data analysis. The program instructions explain how to use the program, how it operates internally, and how to modify it. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 18. MAZE LEARNING (634) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The T maze has been widely used in both animal and human learning experiments for many years. T mazes provide a convenient way to study the speed and accuracy of learning. This experiment presents a T maze consisting of up to 23 choice points. The subject is asked to respond with a left or right turn as rapidly as possible. If the subject does not respond correctly or fast enough, an error is scored and the subject must begin the maze again. The subject must correctly complete the maze a selectable criterion number of times in order complete the experiment. Once the experiment has been completed, there are 36 data analysis options available. The number of errors and correct responses can be displayed or printed, plotted, or recorded for future analysis. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to modify it. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 19. SIGNAL DETECTION (638) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Signal detection theory is a way of characterizing how people make decisions in the face of uncertainty. It has been applied in a wide variety of contexts, ranging from sensory detection experiments to clinical studies. Signal detection theory provides a series of methods and techniques for data analysis which permit the experimenter to separately evaluate two aspects of a subject's performance: his sensitivity, and his response bias. By response bias we mean the effect of the non-sensory factors such as motivation and expectation which affect a subject's response. The Signal Detection program allows the computer system to perform signal detection experiments with many variations. The experiments simulate a vigilance task such as that performed by a radar operator attempting to detect a target aircraft among other aircraft. A detailed description of signal detection theory is beyond the scope of these instructions; however, a clear, concise treatment may be found in: Joan Gay Snodgrass, Theory and Experi-ments in Signal Detection, Baldwin, NY: Life Science Associates, 1972, and in many recent textbooks dealing with perception and psychophysics. The signal detection program presents the subject with a selectable number of trials. On approximately half of the trials, a signal in the form of a selectable symbol is embedded in visual noise (many other selectable symbols). On the other half of the trials, only visual noise is presented. The subject's task is similar to that of a radar operator attempting to locate an aircraft on the radar screen in the presence of many other targets. The number of noise flashes and the duration of the noise flashes may be varied as well as the size of the display. The experimenter may select a set of instructions to the subject designed to minimize misses, to minimize false alarms, or to present neutral instruction. After the experiment is completed, the resulting data may be displayed in an outcome matrix, in a cumulated frequency, probability, and D-prime table, or may be plotted as ROC curves with P(YES/S) (HITS) on the Y axis, and P(YES/N) (FALSE ALARMS) on the X axis. A best fit straight line may be displayed on the screen or a disk file of the data may be made for later analysis or long term storage. The program includes complete instructions that explain its operation and ways of modifying it for special purposes. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 20. PURSUIT ROTOR (640) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The pursuit rotor is a device which has classically been used to study motor learning. The subject attempts to follow a moving stimulus while the device keeps track of the number of errors and time-on and time-off target. As the subject learns the task of following the moving stimulus, errors decrease and time on-target increases. The topic of motor learning and the use of the pursuit rotor is discussed in virtually all textbooks on experimental psychology. A number of representative references are listed in the instructions. The pursuit rotor program allows the computer, in conjunction with a light pen, to function as a pursuit rotor. The light pen is simply a photocell that is connected to the (1) key contacts of the keyboard or to an external interface. The program begins by explaining the use of the pursuit rotor. The experimenter may select the number of trials, the intertrial interval, trial duration, target speed, target size, and pathway size. The program then enters a light pen adjustment phase. During this portion of the program, the experimenter adjusts the sensitivity of the light pen so that it triggers reliably from the blinking spot on the video screen. It is important that the user carefully adjust room illumination, screen brightness, and light pen sensitivity. The program then presents a set of instructions to the subject. Once the subject has read the instructions, the experiment may be started. Each trial consists of a countdown during which reliable triggering of the light pen is assured. At the end of the countdown, the spot of light begins moving around the video screen in a square pattern. The number of errors (light pen off-spot), time on target, and time off target are recorded during each of the trials. After each trial is completed, an intertrial interval precedes the onset of the next trial. After the preselected number of trials has been completed, the experiment enters the data analysis phase. The total number of trials, total number of errors, mean errors per trial, total time, total time on target, and total time off target are displayed. The experimenter then may select either displaying the raw data, displaying a histogram, or making a data recording of either the number of errors for each trial, the time on target for each trial, or the time off target for each trial. A complete set of instructions explains the use, operation, and suggested modifications of the program. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 21. MIRROR TRACING (642) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Mirror tracing experiments have frequently been used to study a subject's ability to learn to reverse normal eye-hand coordination. The subject attempts to follow a pattern with a pencil while viewing the pattern and the pencil in a mirror. Mirror tracing experiments are extensively described in most textbooks on experimental psychology. A list of representative texts is included in the instructions. The mirror tracing program allows the computer to present a simple or complex figure on the video screen. The subject views the pattern and the light pen in a mirror, which is placed at an angle in front of the video screen, and tries to trace out the pattern on the screen as rapidly as possible. The light pen consists of a photocell that may be connected directly to the (1) key keyboard contacts or to an external interface. The computer keeps track of the number of errors, total time per trial, total time on target per trial, and total time off target per trial. After the experiment has been completed, the experimenter may chose to view or print raw data for any of the above measures, draw a histogram of the data, or record the data in a disk file for future analysis. Instructions explain how to use the program, how it operates, and how to modify it. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 22. AUDITORY FREQUENCY DIFFERENCE THRESHOLDS (644) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The difference threshold or difference limen (DL) is one of the most basic and important measures of the performance of a sensory system. It measures the minimum amount of DIFFERENCE between two stimuli that is necessary to permit a subject to judge that they are different on 50% of the trials. The measurement of the difference threshold has both theoretical and practical significance. From a theoretical perspective, it can provide clues to understanding the physiological functioning of sensory systems. Controversies about whether a subject can improve the DL through training, and controversies about which psychophysical method is most appropriate are other related theoretical issues. From a practical standpoint, it can provide knowledge about how well a subject can discriminate between very similar stimuli. For instance, it may be useful to know how well you can discriminate between musical tones if you plan to become a musician. An unusually large difference limen compared to the norms listed in Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology (1951) would indicate that you need a greater difference between tones than most people and might suggest another career choice. The Auditory Frequency Difference Thresholds program is designed to measure the Differenc threshold or difference limen (DL) for auditory frequency tones. The program is exceptionally versatile and can be used for undergraduate instruction or advanced graduate and clinical research. It allows a computer to present and analyze complete experimental determinations of the difference threshold using any of the three classical psychophysical methods. It is written in the simple BASIC computer language and can easily be modified for special applications. The manual includes a complete description of the technical operation of the program, a listing of all commands and variables, and a list of basic and technical references dealing with psychophysics. It should allow an experimenter to understand, modify, and use the program for virtually any type of research. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 23. AUDITORY FREQUENCY SCALING (646) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. Psychophysical scaling is a technique which allows a subject to assign a numerical value to the subjective magnitude of a stimulus. When these subjective judgments are plotted against the physical magnitude of the stimulus, a psychophysical function is produced. The shape of the psychophysical function has been a source of argument for many years. Fechner's law predicts that the psychological judgments are linearly related the logarithm of the stimulus. Fechner thus predicts that plotting the psychological judgments on the vertical axis against the logarithm of the stimulus values on the horizontal axis will yield a straight line. Stevens' law predicts a straight line when the logarithm of the psychological judgments is plotted on the vertical axis and the logarithm of the physical stimulus is plotted on the horizontal axis. Detailed discussions of psychophysical scaling procedures and techniques can be found in virtually all textbooks on experimental psychology and psychophysics. A representative list of such textbooks is attached to the end of the instructions. The Auditory Frequency Scaling program allows the computer system to accept and analyze numerous psychophysical judgments of the frequencies of tones that are presented through the computer's loudspeaker. After a preselected number of trials has elapsed, the data analysis options include drawing a graph of the individual data points or the averaged data points, drawing a graph of the best fit least squares line through these points or drawing a graph of both the points and the best fit line. Each of these three graphs may be drawn on linear, common log, natural log, or exponential axes. If the graphs are drawn with the Y axis plotted in common logarithms, and the X axis plotted linearly, this graph corresponds to Fechner's law ( Y = log X + K ). If the Y axis and the X axis are both plotted as common logarithms, this corresponds to Steven's law ( log Y = log X + K ). The program allows the data to be plotted in both ways and compared for linearity. Other data analysis options include displaying or printing the raw data sequentially on a judgment-by-judgment basis, making a sequential judgment-by-judgment data recording, adding more trials up to a maximum total of 40 (10 judgment) trials, restarting the program at the beginning, or stopping the program. A complete set of instructions explains the use and operation of the program and describes how to make modifications for special applications. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 24. LABORATORY CONTROL SYSTEM (650) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The laboratory control software package allows a microcomputer to control virtually any type of experiment or process, and to record data on all occurrence of events during the experiment. It is designed for use with an external input/output interface. With this program, a single computer can replace the numerous dedicated timers, counters, logic systems, and data recorders normally used for the control and analysis of experiments. The experimental data can be displayed, printed, or saved on disk. The instructions explains the operation of the program and the concept of dividing on experiment into a series of "STATES" (Snapper et. al., 1970). A number of sample psychology experiments have also been included and may be run by simply entering the information when requested at the beginning of the program. The first part of the instructions explains the concept of a STATE. The second part describes the operation of the computer program. The third part presents a set of sample experiments. The fourth part presents a detailed description of program operation and a listing of the computer program. With this information, an experimenter should be able to use the program to run and record data for virtually any type of experiment. Instructions for making a "customized" version of the program to run any specific experiment are also included. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 25. OPERANT CONDITIONING CONTROL PROGRAM (655) Copyright (c) 1995: Thomas B. Perera Ph. D. Montclair State University. and: Thomas B. Perera Jr. MD. - All rights reserved. The operant conditioning control program is designed to convert the computer system into an operant conditioning experiment controller. Ten typical schedules of reinforcement have been programmed and may be selected from an interactive display on the screen of the video monitor. The experimenter may change these schedules at any time during an experiment and response, and reinforcement data are continually displayed and updated on the video screen. A manipulandum, such as a rat lever, voice key, or response button is attached to an external input lead which may be directly connected to the (1) key on the computer keyboard or to an input/output interface. Pressing keyboard key #1 can substitute for activating the #1 input line for test or demonstration purposes. The reinforcement mechanism is connected to the #1 output on the output interface card. Since the operant conditioning control program cannot run every conceivable type of operant conditioning experiment ten of the most widely used schedules of reinforcement have been chosen. Descriptions of these schedules and the behaviors that they may be expected to produce can be found in virtually any textbook on operant conditioning. Several classical references are listed in the instructions. The program routines which are used for each of the schedules of reinforcement are also fully explained in the instructions. These explanations should enable anyone familiar with BASIC language programming to set up and run any desired experiment. For experimenters who are not familiar with BASIC programming, but who want to run, control, and record data from diversified experiments, the laboratory control system software package, based on the SKED system developed by Dr. A. Snapper, is available from Life Science Associates. This software package allows virtually any complex experiment or process to be run by simply dividing the experiment into "states" and specifying the parameters of each state. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 26. DELAYED MATCH-TO-SAMPLE (665) John A. Brendel, Ph.D., Lock Haven University This program is designed for use in undergraduate psychology laboratories and classes. It can be used in classes in Experimental Psychology, Cognition, Perception, Human Information Processing, Memory, or Learning. The delayed match-to-sample task is a recognition memory task in which subjects are asked to recognize information which they have previously seen. The task requires maintenance rehearsal and provides a means of assessing the effects of proactive and retroactive interference on intact short-term memory function. In this program, the subject is presented with one of five stimuli, referred to as the sample on the video terminal. The stimuli may be chosen from one of three categories: letters, numbers, or forms. The sample is observed by the subject for a chosen duration and then disappears. The delay interval then commences, and the experimenter is provided with several options during this delay. The time of the delay can range from 0 to 180 seconds. An interference condition, in which the subject is asked to add pairs of 2 digit numbers, can be presented during the delay. When the delay period is over, three choice stimuli appear on the video terminal. The subject must choose the stimulus that matches the sample. The subject enters his/her choice on the keyboard by pressing the L, M, R, or N key representing left, middle, right, or none, respectively. The subject is told whether he/she is right of wrong (a buzzer sounds on incorrect trials), and the next trial begins. The experimenter may present up to 30 trials. At the end of the experiment, the data for each trial are displayed. Information on trial number, total problems attempted, total correct problems, percentages of correct problems, reaction time in seconds, and match (O or 1) are displayed. The experimenter can specify parameters and conditions from one of six options within the program. In addition, the color, rotation, and scale of the stimuli can be altered by modifying specific lines. The instructions explain internal program operation, how to use the program, and how to modify the program. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 27. LEVELS OF PROCESSING (666) John A. Brendel, Ph.D., Lock Haven University This program acts as an experimenter and enables the user to perform several related experiments to investigate the levels of processing theory of memory. It is designed to be used in an undergraduate laboratory setting in which an IBM-PC or an IBM-PC compatible computer is available. Psychology courses in experimental psychology, cognition, perception, memory, and learning are appropriate for this program. This program presents word lists of selectable length and selectable semantic categorization from a list of 200 words stored in a random access text file. There are twenty semantic categories and a maximum of 10 words in each category. The rate of presentation of the words can be selected by choosing the presentation duration and/or the inter-item interval. In addition, one of 10 different subject instructions can be selected in order to investigate the relationship between subject memorization strategies and free recall performance. Following the presentation of the word list, subjects are asked to recall as many of the words as they can by typing them into the computer. The computer will then display the original word list and the recalled list as well as the percent correct score of the subject. There are ten possible subject instructions which differ in the degree to which they encourage a deep or shallow level of processing. "Search for the Letter " " and "Count Vowels" are examples of shallow processing instructions whereas "Use in a Sentence" and "Memorize" would be examples of deep processing instructions. The instructions explain the use and operation of the program and describe how to make modifications in program operation. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X For further information, and demonstration disks, please contact the publisher: LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES 1 Fenimore Road Bayport, NY 11705 (516) 472-2111 email:lifesciassoc@pipeline.com
Web Pages: http://lifesciassoc.home.pipeline.com
X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X MODULAR LAB SESSIONS FOR PHYSIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X This series of modules, consisting of slides and tape recorded and transcribed commentary, is designed to complement the lectures in an undergraduate or graduate level course in physiological psychology. Each module covers a three hour laboratory session and describes and shows in detail the procedures to be performed by each student group. The students work in pairs, alternating the responsibilities for the specific surgical procedures so that each student becomes competent in the performance of each procedure. Students may carry out the laboratory procedures themselves after observing the slide presentations. The 12 modules have been used, tested and refined for eight years at Barnard College. Each module is supplied with a script, equipment lists, references and names and addresses of equipment and supply vendors. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X MODULE 1: Introduction and Basic Surgical Procedures. Introduction to a physiological psychology laboratory, review of surgical instruments and supplies, anesthetics and their application. Basic surgical procedures that are demonstrated include: calculation and preparation of anesthetic dosage, holding the animal, administering drugs, resuscitation, hair removal, incising, separating skin from underlying tissue, retracting skin, separating muscles, application of antibiotics, suturing, and the use of wound clips. This module is a prerequisite for all other modules employing surgical procedures. (Rat) 38 slides, 46 min. MODULE 2: Tracheal Cannulation. (Prerequisite, Module 1) An ideal "first operation" for students. Includes: positioning the animal on the operating platform; incising the neck; separating tissues and retracting tissues; separating muscle groups; exposing, elevating and incising the trachea; inserting the cannula and tying it in place; repositioning the tissues; and suturing the incision. (Rat) 17 slides, 20 min. MODULE 3: Perfusion of the Circulatory System and Removal of the Central Nervous System. (Prerequisite, Module 2). This third basic procedure module presents a proper and humane method of sacrificing an animal, removing the central nervous system, and preserving it for histological examination. Procedures include: surgical instrument layout, positioning the animal, incising the chest and rib cage, retracting, removal of the heart from the pericardial sac, incising the right auricle, inserting needle into the left ventricle, perfusion, removal of the intact perfused brain and spinal cord, and a procedure for preserving and making them transparent. 18 slides, 21 min. MODULE 4: Exposure of the Sciatic Nerve, Nerve-Muscle Physiology, Removal and Stimulation of a Segment of the Sciatic Nerve, and Clinical and Morphological Observation of Peripheral Nerve Regeneration. (Prerequisite, Module 3). Procedures include: Exposure of the sciatic nerve in the rat's thigh, stimulation of the nerve in situ, study of nerve-muscle physiology, comparing electrical stimulation of the nerve with direct electrical stimulation of the muscle, determining the frequency of stimulation necessary to produce tetany in the muscle, removal of a section of the sciatic nerve, electrical stimulation , impulse propagation, observation of individual axons in the nerve with the naked eye or under a microscope. After two weeks, re-opening the incision shows numerous regenerating nerve fibers readily visible to the naked eye. Coverage includes: incising the skin. locating the sciatic nerve, stimulation of the nerve in situ, removal of a segment of the nerve, nerve action potential recording, direct and microscopic observation of individual axons in the nerve. suturing the incision, testing for neurological deficits, reopening the incision, and observation of regenerated nerve fibers. (Rat) 34 slides, 25 min. MODULE 5: Observation of Image Formation and Dissection of the eye. The anatomy of the eye is studied utilizing readily available bovine eyes. Considerably larger than human eyes, these specimens allow students to observe the anatomy of the eye directly. A window is cut through the side of the eye and an interface allows observation of the actual formation of a visual image on the retina. Following this, the eye is dissected and the important structures are shown. Bovine Eye) 21 slides, 24 min. MODULE 6: Direct Observation of a Subject's Retina and External Ear, Plotting the Blind Spot, and Direct Observation of One's Own Blind Spot and Retina. Each student makes two concentric plots of their blind spot. Then, a special technique using a flashlight allows each student to look directly at his own blind spot. Placing their heads in the same position used in plotting the blind spot, the actual image of the blind spot is superimposed and compared for accuracy. Finally, ophthalmoscopes and an otoscope are used to examine each other's eyes and external ears. The diagnostic importance of the appearance of each is briefly discussed. (Human Subjects) 16 slides, 20 min. MODULE 6a: Production of a "Stabilized Retinal Image". This simplified demonstration includes instructions and materials for a demonstration of phenomena associated with the "Stabilized Retinal Image". Complete adaptation to the image takes place in less than 2 seconds. The demonstration includes a penlight flashlight and alkaline batteries. (Human Subjects) MODULE 7: Exposure of the Spinal Cord, Stimulation of Spinal Nerves, and Removal of the Entire Intact Central Nervous System. (Prerequisites, Modules 1,2,3) The student exposes and stimulates the spinal cord and spinal nerves of a rat. Specificity of functions of dorsal and ventral spinal nerve roots is demonstrated. The animal is then sacrificed and the entire central nervous system including the brain and spinal cord is removed intact, A simple procedure allows the central nervous system to be made transparent to facilitate observation of internal structures. Coverage includes: incising the skin, separating muscles, removing muscles, removal of spinal processes, exposure of the spinal cord, exposure and stimulation of spinal nerve roots. (Rat) 26 slides, 23 min. MODULE 8: Removal of the Lamb Brain from the Skull and Dissection of the Brain. Students remove the lamb brain from the skull and observe the locations and paths of the cranial nerves and the lobes of a brain not too different in size or shape from a human brain. Ordinary carpentry tools are used to open and remove the skull from the brain. Once the brain and brainstem are removed, they are sectioned on the midline. One hemisphere is made transparent for easy inspection of the subcortical structures and the other hemisphere is sectioned and the exposed structures identified. (Lamb head) 34 slides, 25 min. MODULE 9: The Anatomy of the Human Brain. Utilizing coronal sections, and saggital sections of an actual human brain, students learn the location of cortical and sub-cortical structures. Sequential sections of several human brains are shown. Students construct a 3-dimensional model of the brain out of cardboard. The brain model may be traced directly onto cardboard from the slides, or MODULE 9a, a kit of materials and instructions, may be used by each student. (MODULE 9 includes one copy of MODULE 9a). (Human Brain and brain model) 31 slides, 35 min. MODULE 9a: Individual Materials and Instructions for the Construction of a 3-Dimensional Model of the Human Brain. This module includes the printed diagrams and backing material to construct a 3-dimensional model of the human brain. The model includes a mid-saggital section, several coronal sections, ventricles, the limbic system, the thalamus, the lenticular nucleus, and the caudate nucleus (Human brain model kit) MODULE 10: Stimulating and Lesioning the Rat Brain with Clinical and Gross Histological Follow-Up. (Prerequisite, Modules 1,2,3). The surface of the brain is electrically stimulated and gross motor responses are observed. A lesion is made in the brain and the animal is sutured. One week after recovery, tests are performed for behavioral deficits. The animal is sacrificed (using techniques learned in MODULE 3), and the brain and spinal cord is removed intact. The brain and spinal cord are made transparent and the site of the lesion is observed. (Rat) 27 slides, 23 min. MODULE 11: Methodology and Techniques for Stereotaxically Implanting Electrodes into the Hypothalamus of the Rat, the Production of Hypothalamically-Induced Eating Behavior through Stimulation of the Hypothalamus, and Procedures for the Histological Verification of Electrode Loci. (Prerequisites, Modules 1,2,3). The step-by-step procedures and the apparatus involved in implanting electrodes into the lateral hypothalamus of the rat brain are presented. Stimulus-bound eating is produced through electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus. Finally, the techniques for removing the brain from the perfused rat and sectioning the brain for histological verification of the electrode loci are shown. (Rat) 44 slides, 35 min. MODULE 12: Biofeedback and Recording of Electrophysiological Signals from Humans. The techniques involved in recording and feedback to the subject, the Galvanic skin response, electrocardiogram, electroencephalogram, and electromyogram are presented in detail. Electrode application and electronic amplification are discussed. Typical electrophysiological responses are presented and described. Subjects can rapidly learn to control internal electrophysiological responses through the use of biofeedback. (Human subjects) 30 slides, 32 min. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Individual modules or the complete course are available from the publisher: LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES 1 Fenimore Road Bayport, NY 11705 Email: lifesciassoc@pipeline.com
Web Pages: http://lifesciassoc.home.pipeline.com
` The complete course includes 336 slides, cassette recordings & transcripts. Slides, tape recordings, and transcripts are available separately. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X SEQUENTIAL GROSS CORONAL SLICES OF THE HUMAN BRAIN These two sets of slides consist of 10 color pnotographs of coronal slices of two formalin-perfused human brains. Each slice is approximately 1.5 cm. thick and is displayed in relation to the entire brain. The slides may be used to illustrate and clarify lectures or laboratory sessions which deal with the gross anatomy of the human brain. Small sections of several slices have been removed by the pathology department of the hospital from which the brains were obtained, but this does not interfere with the visibility of any of the structures in the brain. Sets A and B were made in the same way, but are from two different brains, and can be compared to reveal individual differences. Brain A or Brain B or both may be ordered from the publisher. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X SEQUENTIAL CORONAL SECTIONS OF THE BRAIN OF A BABOON This series of slides was made from sequential coronal sections of the brain of an adult baboon. It is very similar to the human brain and may be used to illustrate or clarify lectures on the anatomy of the brain. The sections are 30 microns thick and have been made 1 mm. apart. They have been stained with Nissi stain. The entire set may be used for detailed study, as in an anatomy course or every other slide or every third slide may be used for less detailed coverage. The complete set of 48 slides or sets of 24, or 16 slides may be ordered from the publisher (LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES). X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X THE ELECTRICAL STIMULATION OF THE BRAIN Part 1: Methodology and Techniques for Implanting Electrodes This series of slides and commentary covers the step-by-step procedures involved in the implantation of electrodes and the electrical stimulation of the feeding center in the lateral hypothalamic nucleus of the rat. Electrical stimulation of this nucleus produces stimulus-bound eating behavior which continues for as long as the electrical stimulus is applied. All of the important methodology and procedures are explained and shown in detail. Coverage includes: the stereotaxic atlas, anesthesia, general and specialized surgical instruments, the stereotaxic instrument, placement of the animal in the stereotaxic instrument, surgical procedures, mounting the electrical connector, stimulation of the hypothalamus, producing stimulus-bound eating, and the extreme enlargement of the stomach produced by prolonged stimulation. Part 1. 36 slides, script, reprint, with or without cassette of commentary X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Part 2: Conditioned Suppression of Hypothalamically Induced Eating, Perfusion, and Histological Verification of Electrode Loci Electrical stimulation of the feeding center in the lateral hypothalamic nucleus produces stimulus-bound eating and gross distention of the stomach. A conditioning procedure involving the conditioned suppression paradigm produces complete suppression of the eating behavior in the presence of the hypothalamic stimulation. The behavioral tests are shown and the detailed procedure for perfusing the rat's circulatory system with formalin, the stereotaxic and surgical procedures involved in the removal of the brain, and the sectioning of the brain for histological verification of the electrode loci are also shown and described. A reprint of an article from the Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology presenting the results of the study is included. Thus, a student can trace the evolution of a typical research project in physiological psychology from its conception to the final published report. Part 2. 25 slides, script, reprint, with or without cassette of commentary X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X CAREERS IN PSYCHOLOGY Clinical Psychology Herbert H. Krauss, Hunter College Perception Frank J. Mandriota, Hunter College Physiological Psychology Thomas B. Perera, Barnard College Learning Ellen & Thomas Reese, Mount Holyoke College Social-Personality Charles P. Smith, City University of New York Biopsychology Robert L. Thompson, Hunter College : Applied Psychology Lawrence R. Zeitlin, Bernard Baruch College This tape-slide set introduces the beginning student to the contents of the major sub-divisions within contemporary psychology. Each of the divisions is covered in 13-15 minutes with correlated slides. The slides may be advanced manually or by the synchronization signals on the tape. The authors have provided a representative overview of their fields in an interesting and cogent manner. This unit is ideal for the first meeting of an introductory course. It can also be presented in parts at appropriate times in a course, or used for individual study. Careers in Psychology consists of approximately 245 slides and severn cassettes. Each unit is also available separately from the publisher: LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES 1 Fenimore Road Bayport, NY 11705 (516) 472-2111 Email: lifesciassoc@pipeline.com
Web Pages: http://lifesciassoc.home.pipeline.com
` X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X MEASUREMENT OF DARK ADAPTATION This set of materials will enable students or researchers to generate dark adaptation functions for individual subjects or groups of subjects. The set may be used for a laboratory session in a course in Sensation and Perception, as a class-room demonstration, or for research purposes. A small red fixation point is hung in front of a projection screen and the stimuli are flashed on the screen adjacent to the fixation point. Stimuli are produced by 41 neutral density filters mounted in standard 2 x 2 slides. The slides are provided in a Kodak Carousel tray. Included are: A 110V red fixation light, carousel tray, 100 sheets of graph paper and complete instructions for gathering the data. Using ascending method of limits trials, the intensity of each flash may be increased by 0.1 log unit until detection occurs. Variables which can be manipulated include: retinal location, preadapting intensity and duration, and visual angle of the stimulus. Additional materials required are a Kodak Carousel slide projector and a projection screen. A completely dark room must be used. X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X For further information, and demonstration disks, please contact the publisher: LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES
***LIFE SCIENCE ASSOCIATES IS NO LONGER ACTIVE***
1 Fenimore Road Bayport, NY 11705 (516) 472-2111 Email: lifesciassoc@pipeline.com
Web Pages: http://lifesciassoc.home.pipeline.com
` X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X


CONTACT INFORMATION

Please Note: IF YOU DO NOT RECEIVE AN ANSWER TO YOUR EMAIL
IT MEANS THAT I CAN NO LONGER RESPOND.
Please use internet search engines to find other information if you do not receive a reply.

Professor Tom Perera Ph. D. - W1TP
Professor Emeritus:
Montclair State University
  • Go to Tom Perera's Professional Neuropsychology Home Page.
    • Email Address: (To help me avoid automated 'junk mail' programs,
      I ask you to type my email address as follows with no spaces between words:)

    • PLEASE TYPE: keys
    • THEN TYPE THE @ SYMBOL
    • THEN TYPE: w1tp.com

    • Please NOTE: { it is w1tp - W then the NUMERAL "1" (one) then tp }... (It is NOT wLtp or wItp).

    • Please NOTE: IF YOU DO NOT RECEIVE AN ANSWER TO YOUR EMAIL IT MEANS THAT I CAN NO LONGER RESPOND.
      Please use internet search engines to find other information if you do not receive a reply.

    • Please NOTE: You MUST include the word KEYS in the Email Subject Line.
    • Please Sign your email.
      IF you do not receive a reply my spam cleaner might have intercepted your email.
      Pease try again with a different Subject and Text.

    • ( Please Enquire Before Sending Attachments Larger Than 1MB - Thank you.)
    Internet On-Line Telegraph & Scientific Instrument Museums:
    http://w1tp.com

    Internet ENIGMA Museum:
    https://EnigmaMuseum.com

    COPYRIGHT NOTICE: (Copyright (c) 2023: Prof. Tom Perera Ph. D.)
    Although all the pictures and text are copyrighted, you may use any of them for your own personal applications including public lectures and demonstrations, publications and websites as long as you mention the w1tp.com Museum. If you plan to offer them for sale to the public in any form, please email me for permission which I will generally grant as long as you mention my museum: http://w1tp.com or https://EnigmaMuseum.com My email address is given at the bottom of this page. Some of the material may require contacting other copyright owners for commercial use and I will inform you by email. Please also see the Disclaimer of Warranty.